How the Brain Relies on Sight in the Absence of Touch
Somatosensation, the combined sense of touch and proprioception, is
integral to how we navigate our body in space. We are aware of our
embodiment even when we close our eyes. But what happens when we lack
this sensory experience from our body, and how does our brain adapt? How
do other senses, notably sight, compensate to generate a sense of the
body? Researchers recently had that opportunity to study two unique
individuals without somatosensation, one case acquired, the other congenital. Ian from the U.K. developed a complete loss of his sense of
touch and proprioception (the awareness of our body's position and
movement) below the neck at age 19 after what is believed to be an
autoimmune response after illness; Ian required months of rehabilitation
to learn to move his body and limbs again. Kim from the U.S., on the
other hand, was born without sensory nerve fibers to feel her body,
consequently possessing neither touch sensation nor proprioception.
For
the study, Ian and Kim, along with age-matched controls, participated
in experiments designed to assess their mental image of
their bodies and their unconscious sense of their bodies in space. The
researchers found that Kim's representation of her hand was closer to
that of the controls in that both were distorted; for the controls, this
is due to a distorted sensory map. Ian, by contrast, had a more accurate
representation of his hand compared to Kim and the controls. The
investigators hypothesize that the greater accuracy is because Ian's
hands are always under his conscious control. In an experiment related
to body schema, the researchers looked at how the participants reacted
when a visual target appeared near their hand, i.e., within their
peri-personal space. Those with intact proprioception would react
rapidly. They found that Kim's results closely matched the controls
compared to Ian's, suggesting that Kim has an unconscious representation
of her body despite never having been able to feel it. This is similar
to the way a person will instinctively duck when a ball
flies toward them, even before consciously processing what the object
is. Ian, however, relies on much slower processes involving conscious
visual perception. The differences between Ian's and Kim's conditions
highlight how conscious compensatory mechanisms developed in adulthood
after sensory loss differ from the neuronal reorganization that might
occur in a person born without that sense. However, whether
unconsciously or consciously, we all arrive at a sense of self. As one
of the researchers concludes, "What we can learn from this is that you
might not do it in the way that
others do it, but you will find a way to make a body schema. You will
find a way to make a sense of yourself."
Interhemispheric Transfer of Visual Working Memory
When we shift our gaze around to look at a scene, or even temporarily
turn away, we rely on visual working memory to retain what we see in
mind. The ability to hold that memory enables volitional control of our
actions, allowing us to decide to react to something now or later. This
visual working memory is a cognitively interesting feat, given that our
left visual hemifields correspond to our right cerebral hemisphere, and
our right visual hemifields correspond to our left cerebral hemisphere.
New research by neuroscientists at MIT, using animal primate models,
found that when an object shifts across our field of
view, either because it moved or because our eyes did, the brain immediately
transfers a memory of it to the opposite (contralateral) brain hemisphere, in particular, to a group of neurons in the prefrontal cortex.
Remarkably, even though the new group of neurons encodes the object in
the new position, the brain continues to recognize it as the same object
that had been in the other hemisphere's field of view. That being said,
results in monkeys indicate decreased performance in cases where the
monkeys had to shift their gaze, suggesting that shifting gaze requires
extra cerebral processing. This switch from one side of one's field of
view to the other is accompanied by a signature change in the rhythm of
brain wave frequencies to transfer the memory information from one side
of the brain to the other. The news article explains, "As
the transfer occurred, the synchrony across
hemispheres of very low frequency 'theta' waves (~4-10 Hz) and high
frequency 'beta' waves (~17-40 Hz) rose and the synchrony of
'alpha/beta' waves (~11-17 Hz) declined." The lead author of the study
adds, “This
is another form of gating. This time alpha/beta is gating the memory
transfer between hemispheres.” Perhaps the most surprising discovery
from the experiments was that the
prefrontal cortex uses different neurons to encode the memory of an
object depending on whether the object (in the same spot of the visual
field) was initially seen at that location or transferred there from the
other hemisphere. The scientific implication of that finding is the
idea that even the same information could still be encoded by different, arbitrarily assembled ensembles of neurons.
Radiology Insights into COVID-19 Ocular Abnormalities
The COVID-19 pandemic has affected the entire globe since its onset in
early 2020, including impact in all specialties of healthcare. Within
ophthalmology, SARS-CoV-2 has been linked with ocular complications from
conjunctivitis to retinopathy. Insight from radiology provides
additional clues of ocular abnormalities in individuals with severe
COVID-19. A study by researchers at the French Society of Neuroradiology
(SFNR), from a sample of 129 patients with severe COVID-19 who
underwent brain MRI, found that nine of those patients, eight of whom
had spent time in the intensive care unit (ICU) for COVID-19, had
abnormal MRI findings of the globe. Specifically, the MRI scans showed
one or more nodules in the posterior pole, the first time these findings
have been described using MRI. The nodules occurred in the macular
region in all nine patients, eight of whom showed a bilateral
presentation and two of whom additionally had nodules beyond the macula.
The mechanism of nodule formation is unknown, although the researchers
speculate a relation to inflammation triggered by the virus as well as
inadequate ocular venous drainage while prone or intubated during
hospitalization. This study aligns with other research showing that
COVID-19 has greater impact on those with preexisting health conditions;
among the nine patients with eye nodules, two had diabetes, six were
obese, and two had hypertension. Eight of the nine patients were men.
The researchers will monitor the COVID-19 survivors to see if there is
any visual impairment from the nodules. The first author of the study
comments that because patients with severe COVID-19 are often being
treated for much more severe, life-threatening conditions, eye problems
could go unnoticed in the clinic. He states, “Our study advocates for
screening of all patients hospitalized in
the ICU for severe COVID-19. We believe those
patients should receive specific eye-protective treatments.”
Additionally, the researchers will conduct a prospective study with
dedicated high-resolution MRI to explore the eye and orbit in patients
with mild to moderate COVID-19.
Archeologists Examine a Color-Matching Device
Color perception, something most of us take for granted, is ultimately a
subjective and individual experience. The ability to perceive and
identify colors, however, becomes functionally relevant in some
professions, such as interior design and cosmetology, or even archeology
as in this case. Because color perception is subjective, scientists
were excited when a handheld color-matching gadget called the X-Rite
Capsure came on the market. Made by the same company that owns Pantone,
they had hoped that it would offer a consistent way of determining
color, free of human bias as well as variations in lighting, sample
quality, and observer perspective. However, a study by archeologists, to
whom the device was marketed to identify the color of artifacts and
soil samples, found that the X-Rite Capsure often misreads colors
readily distinguished by the human eye when tested against the Munsell
color
system, the current archeological standard for identifying colors using a
binder of 436 unique color chips. The study specifically tested the
Capsure's readings for all 436 color chips as well as 140 pottery
briquettes for the three elements of Munsell’s system: a color’s general
family (hue), intensity (chroma), and lightness (value). The details of
how the X-Rite Capsure differed from human observers in determining
color can be found in the articles. These differences are not
functionally trivial, however, since identifying subtle differences in
color gives information about a sample's composition, origins, and
history of use. Highlighting a device that is functionally relevant
within the field of archeology, the researchers argue, “We need to pay
really close attention and record how we’re describing color in order to
make good data. Ultimately, if we’re putting bad color data in, we’re
going to get bad data out.” Because the device is nonetheless internally
consistent in its color identifications, work remains to optimize the
advantages of such devices and artificial intelligence to match human
perception, arguably difficult for a quality as complex as color.
Literacy Screening Tool Developed for Young Children
“By screening early during pediatric
clinic visits, especially in practices serving disadvantaged families,
we can hopefully target effective interventions that help children
better prepare for kindergarten and improve reading outcomes,” the author of a book called The Reading House
offers. The researchers at Cincinnati Children's Hospital developed the
book (series) as a literacy screening tool for children ages 3 to 5 to
facilitate the early identification of reading difficulties in primary
care and preschool settings. The present study, involving 70 healthy
children (34 boys and 36 girls) between 3
and 5 years of age from various socioeconomic backgrounds, sought to
validate the book as a literacy screening tool. The
children completed standardized assessments of literacy
skills related to vocabulary, rhyming and rapid automatized naming.
Fifty-two of these children also completed
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), including measurement of cortical
thickness, with thicker cortices (especially in the left hemisphere
involved with language processing) being predictive of better reading
outcomes. They found thicker gray matter cortex in the cerebral left
hemisphere in children with higher TRH scores. Higher TRH scores were
also strongly correlated with higher vocabulary,
rhyming and rapid naming scores, according to the researchers. Finally,
the researchers found that children from lower-SES households had a less
mature ("strained") cortical pattern and thinner cortices overall
compared to children from higher-SES households.
In Other News
(1) Color vision in butterflies
(2) Comprehensive eye exams before going back to school
(3) Can an experimental eye disease drug treat COVID-19 associated lung problems?