Tripartite Neuron Circuit Governs Representation Consistency in the Mouse Visual Cortex
Visual processing in the brain is accompanied by a lot of noise as
different groups of neurons in the visual cortex (V1) are stimulated by
the same scene. Neuroscientists at MIT explored how the brain
compensates for this neuronal noise to produce fidelity in visual
representation. The researchers used optogenetics and two-photon
microscopy to observe the brain activity of mice watching movies. They
discovered that in addition to groups of excitatory pyramidal neurons
that respond when images appear, two groups of inhibitory neurons work
in a circuit to enforce reliability behind the scenes. In
particular, low representation reliability corresponded with high
activity
among parvalbumin-expressing (PV) inhibitory neurons and
low activity among somatostatin-expressing (SST) neurons. High
representation
reliability corresponded with low PV activity and high SST activity.
When excitatory activity became unreliable, SST activity followed PV
activity.
The PV neurons inhibit excitatory
activity to control their gain, that is, to prevent them from becoming
saturated amid a flood of incoming images. However, gain suppression
comes at the cost of making the representation of images less reliable.
Meanwhile, the SST neurons can inhibit the activity of PV neurons, the
SST neurons kicking in when excitatory activity has become unreliable. This
tripartite neuron circuit was then modeled with computer software. Once
they learned how the circuit worked, the researchers could control the
inhibitory cells to influence how consistently the excitatory neurons
represented images. For example, increasing PV activity made any
existing reliability less reliable, and increasing SST activity made
unreliable activity more reliable. Importantly, because SST inhibitory
neurons work by inhibiting PV neurons—in other words, SST neurons
increase reliability by inhibiting inhibition—they would not be able to
enforce reliability without PV neurons. It is an inherent tripartite
circuit, due in part to differences in how these two cells form
synapses. (SST neurons form inhibitory synapses at neuron dendrites
while PV neurons form inhibitory synapses at neuron somas.) The authors
remark, "[I]t
is the co-operative dynamics
between SST and PV [neurons] which is important for controlling the
temporal fidelity of sensory processing." They add that the circuit also
interacts and receives input from other higher order brain areas, for
example, to concentrate on a particular scene with volitional attention.
Binocular Connectivity with Selective Monocular Function in the Retinogeniculate Pathway
The visual thalamus—in humans, comprised of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN), and the pulvinar—relays
visual stimuli from the retina to the primary visual cortex. The
retinogeniculate pathway, involving the LGN, is of greatest interest to
visual perception. According to a long-standing theory, signals from the
two eyes are separated in the visual thalamus before being transmitted
to the visual cortex. However, investigations into the visual thalamus
of mice showed that a large number of neurons form synapses with retinal
ganglion cells (RGCs) of both eyes. Using optogenetics, researchers
confirmed this finding. However, they also discovered that very few of
the neurons actually receive equal signal strength from both eyes.
Rather, in most neurons, signal strength from one eye dominates over the
other, with the weaker signals from the non-dominant eye not reaching
threshold to trigger an action potential. The researchers hypothesize
that they do not play a major role in processing visual information. In
other words, although demonstrating structural binocular connectivity,
these cells in the mouse visual thalamus were functionally monocular,
resolving the contradictory findings in the earlier study. From a basic
science standpoint, the findings showed that anatomy does not
necessarily confer function; in this case, neurons in contact with one
another do not necessarily communicate extensively. From the perspective
of function, the study showed that even with equal access to both eyes,
thalamic cells selectively establish functional connections with only
one eye. Monocular connections are explained by synaptic selection
rather than by anatomical overlap between RGC axons and dLGN neuron
dendrites alone. Furthermore, synapses with the dominant eye strengthen
while those of the nondominant eye remain immature. The scientists next
plan to study how eye dominance is determined, whether there are
variations in different RGC types, and whether the immature contact
sites could be activated when needed, for example to treat eye
conditions such as amblyopia.
Automated Pupillometry Improves Care in Neuro ICU
Assessing pupil size and reactivity provides vital information about the
care of patients in the neurology ICU. Although manual methods based on
visual estimates via penlight remain established practice, in large
part due to convenience and equipment availability, they also present
obstacles in terms of reliability, varying among operators and patient
eye colors. Intubation and intravenous lines blocking a clear line of
sight, as well as cases of sedation and pharmacologic paralysis, can
also present challenges. The Neurology Department at Vanderbilt University Medical Center is assessing the use of an automated handheld
pupillometer by NeurOptics for more accurate and objective measurements
of pupillary size, shape, and light reactivity. These measurements are
quantified as a Neurological Pupil index (NPi), which provides valuable
information regarding subtle changes before visible changes in pupil
size are seen. As an assistant professor of the program remarks, “The
impact of accurate versus slightly inaccurate serial measurement can be
profound...Pupillometry may allow for more urgent
medical or surgical intervention in some of our sickest patients,
particularly those with rising intracranial pressures that can result in
permanent damage, like brain herniation.” Similarly, they report that
quantitative pupillometry outperformed manual measurements in comatose
cardiac arrest patients, especially in predicting unfavorable outcomes,
as well as successfully identified early changes in neurologic function,
intracranial pressure, and treatment response to osmotherapy, among
other neurologic conditions. Automated pupillometry, even at VUMC, is
not indicated for all patients, but rather for patients with the highest
risk, such as patients with intracranial injury or patients who
experience neurologic
decline. The team anticipates automated pupillometers becoming standard
of care for units that treat trauma and critical illness.
Representational Drift in Neurons of the Visual Cortex
It was previously thought that neurons of the visual cortex reliably
encode information from sensory stimuli. However, experiments in mice
found evidence of "representational drift" of neuronal activity, known
to occur in other areas of the brain, to also happen in the visual
cortex (V1). Furthermore, while flexibility in neuronal activity is
expected in response to changes in learning or experience, what was
surprising was that neurons in the visual cortex exhibited drift even
when presented with the same stimulus across time. The experiments
involved showing mice a short 30-second movie clip
on a loop, while the activity of hundreds of neurons were recorded in
their primary visual cortex using two-photon calcium imaging. These
viewing sessions were repeated weekly for up to seven weeks. Analysis of
the neuronal population activity showed that individual neurons did not
respond the same way to the visual stimulus, i.e., the exact scene at
specific moments in the film, when the mouse watched the film one week
as compared with another week. Specifically, the single-neuron
activities consisted of individual spiking episodes, and it is these
spiking episodes that showed distinct variations across weeks. In other
words, the long-term stability of single-neuron responses varied across
the weeks. As the first author of the study argues, “What is somewhat
unexpected is that even when there is no learning, or
no experience changes, neural activity still changes across days in
different brain areas.” Although an implicit variable that is difficult
to dissociate is the learning and experience changes that occur across
the weeks beyond the time periods when activity is recorded in the lab,
underlying the concept of representational drift is the thought that
representations, or identities, of stimuli are expected to remain stable
across the life of the organism. Discovering that neuronal encoding of
stimuli varies across time could point to compensatory mechanisms in
downstream brain areas to maintain stable representations.
Framing Strategies Stabilize Visual Perception
When
we move our eyes to look around a scene, the image of the world
projected onto our retina also moves, which in turn changes the image
that is sent to the brain. Yet, our brains perceive a stabilized view of
the world. Psychologists studying this process, known as "visual stabilization," tested the brain's inherent "steady cam" through two
experiments, conducted both in person and online. The first experiment
is an example of “paradoxical stabilization,” in this case
demonstrating how a stabilized view is produced by a moving frame
replicated on a computer monitor. A second experiment
demonstrated that participants perceived the location of objects in
relation to (moving) frames, even when the objects themselves were
stationary. This effect held true across a wide range of frame speeds,
sizes, and path lengths tested. In other words, even though the image of
what we see "moves" on our retina when our eyes move, (1) the presence
of a frame in the scene stabilizes our judgments of location, and (2)
our brain
has a tendency to perceive objects in relation to available frames. As
the senior author of the study states, “Our results show that a framing
strategy is at work behind the scenes
all the time, which helps stabilize our visual experience.”
Social Interaction as Seen through Pupils
As part of the autonomic sympathetic nervous system, the pupils of our
eyes change size in response to arousing emotional stimuli.
Psychologists in China were interested in exploring whether pupil size
could also serve as a window to social motivations, particularly social
interaction. In a set of three experiments, the scientists tested
pupillary response to second-person social interaction, observation of
third-person social interaction, and communicative social interaction.
They found that pupil size enlarged when the participants perceived a
single agent sending interactive intention toward them as compared to
toward others, and when they viewed two agents interacting with each
other as compared to facing away from each other. These pupil dilation
effects, however, relied on correct understanding of the communicative
intentions of the interacting agents. The experiments show that, perhaps
related to well-documented relationships between pupils and emotional
response, pupil size could also indicate perception of social
interaction. In brief, humans are social beings possessing intrinsic
motivations toward interacting with others and maintaining social
relationships. The study adds insight to the visual system's sensitivity
to social interaction, with pupils being a potential biomarker for
early detection of social cognitive disorders.
In Other News
(1) NJIT secures $3.7 million to study concussion-induced eye disorder (Related)
(2) Hippocampus could help guide visuospatial attention and short-term memory
(3) Diversity in pupil shapes of frogs and toads
Saturday, October 2, 2021
Week in Review: Number 34
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment